Risk: A Practical Guide for Deciding What's Really Safe and What's Really Dangerous in the World Around You

Home
<< Book   Book >> click this image for more info on: Risk: A Practical Guide for Deciding What's Really Safe and What's Really Dangerous in the World Around You
Risk: A Practical Guide for Deciding What's Really Safe and What's Really Dangerous in the World Around You

by: David Ropeik, George Gray

Topics include: hazardous waste recycling program, cell phone radiation, women with implants, particle pollution, medical waste incinerators, mobile phone use, municipal waste incinerators, foodborne illness, safety belt use, air toxics

More info & price

First Sentence:
WE LIVE in a dangerous world.

This book was based on research gleaned from:
Environmental Protection Agency, Low Medium High, Harvard School, National Academy of Sciences, National Safety Council, National Cancer Institute, World Health Organization, World War, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, United Kingdom, National Institutes of Health, Harvard Medical School, American Cancer Society, Pennsylvania Avenue, Assistant Professor, Center Drive, Institute of Medicine, Clifton Road Atlanta, Nobel Prize, Associate Professor, Gradient Corporation, Spring Lake Drive Itasca, Blood Institute, Los Angeles, National Heart AssocPress Reviews:From Publishers Weekly
For those who can't find enough time in a day to worry about all of life's possible dangers, there's a new book to help them prioritize. Risk: A Practical Guide for Deciding What's Really Safe and What's Really Dangerous in the World Around You is a fascinating assessment of the level of threat posed by various illnesses, accidents, environmental pollutants and other factors. David Ropeik, director of risk communication at the Harvard Center for Risk Analysis, and his colleague George Gray, a toxicologist, evaluate such real or perceived menaces as cell phones, biological weapons, pesticides, mad cow disease and medical errors. For each entry, they analyze the potential hazards and offer tips for reducing risk. They also include a "Risk Meter"-a chart that shows likelihood of exposure and severity of consequences at a glance. 25 b&w illus. Copyright 2002 Reed Business Information, Inc. Review
"An endlessly fascinating reference book, to be consulted occasionally in time of need and in time of curiosity." Review
"An endlessly fascinating reference book, to be consulted occasionally in time of need and in time of curiosity." About the Author
David Ropeik is the director of risk communications at the Harvard Center for Risk Analysis. A regular commentator on NPR, he has written for the Boston Globe and worked as a television journalist for twenty-two years. He lives in Concord, Massachusetts. George Gray, a toxicologist, lectures on risk analysis and directs a program on food and agriculture at the Harvard Center for Risk Analysis. He lives in Belmont, Massachusetts.

Excerpt. Reprinted by permission. All rights reserved. INTRODUCTION "I"ve developed a new philosophy . . . I only dread one day at a time." Charlie Brown We live in a dangerous world. Yet it is also a world far safer in many ways than it has ever been. Life expectancy is up. Infant mortality is down. Diseases that only recently were mass killers have been all but eradicated. Advances in public health, medicine, environmental regulation, food safety, and worker protection have dramatically reduced many of the major risks we faced just a few decades ago.
Yet new risks have arisen. Hazardous waste. Nuclear power. Genetically modified foods. Mad cow disease. Ozone depletion. Artificial sweeteners. For all the unquestionable benefits of the modern technological world and its scientific power, the march of progress that has given us longer, healthier lives has subjected us to new perils.
We often react to this conflict, of progress on the one hand and risk on the other, with fear. Most of us are more afraid than we have ever been. And not just from any single risk that happens to be grabbing the headlines at a given point in time, whether it"s terrorism or West Nile virus. We are afraid, cumulatively, of all the new bogeymen to which our modern existence has exposed us. Many polls find that people feel the world today is more dangerous for humans than it has ever been.
It is true that the industrial and information ages have spawned a whole new range of risks, and raised awareness of those that were lurking all the time. But research suggests that our fears may not match the facts. We may be too afraid of lesser risks and not concerned enough about bigger ones. Polls show a wide gap between what the public and the "experts" think is actually dangerous and what is considered relatively safe. Who"s right? There are no simple answers. But information can help us begin to sort things out. Some basic facts about the risks we face, or think we face, can help us make more sense of just what we need to worry about. The intent of this book is to provide that information. We want to empower you to make better judgments about how to protect yourself and your family and friends. Our goal is to help you put the risks you face into perspective. Risk issues are often emotional. They are contentious. Disagreement is often deep and fierce. This is not surprising, given that how we perceive and respond to risk is, at its core, about nothing less than survival. The perception of and response to danger is a powerful and fundamental driver of human behavior, thought, and emotion.
In writing this book, we tried to stay as neutral about these controversial issues as we could. We think that information devoid of advocacy is a tough commodity to come by these days, and will be more useful to you. We do not tell you what you should think. Nor do we make judgments about whether a risk is big or small for you as an individual. We offer numbers for society as a whole, but there is no overarching single conclusion about any risk that can be drawn for each reader. Each of you has unique circumstances that make any given risk higher or lower for you than it might be for the next person. Ultimately, how you perceive a given risk is a decision for you to make in the context of your own life. We simply hope that you are more able to make more informed choices after reading the information we present. As Arthur Conan Doyle wrote in The Hound of the Baskervilles, "That which is clearly known hath less terror than that which is but hinted at and guessed." We have gathered and analyzed the basic information available on major risk issues and synthesized from all that research a fair presentation that you can use to make up your mind about the risks we examine. Of course we have made judgments along the way, about which risks to include or omit, about what information to offer and what information to leave out. But we have done so in an honest effort to get to the basic core truths about each risk as we see it, in as fair a way as possible. You may well disagree with some of the judgments we"ve made. That"s a risk we run in taking on a subject fraught with so much emotion. We encourage you to use this book in two ways. Reading it all the way through will let you see how each risk compares with the others and will help you put them all in perspective. There are a lot of statistics in this book. They are provided to give you an idea of how big or small each individual risk might be. But they will also let you compare similar statistics for various risks from chapter to chapter. Together, these numbers should help you gain a larger view of many of the risks you face. But we also encourage you to use this guide as you would an encyclopedia, as a reference work you will turn to over time, whenever there"s something about a particular risk you want to know. Each chapter, for example, begins with a useful explanation of the specific hazard: What is radon? How do air bags or nuclear power plants or cell phones work? What are the most common forms of sexually transmitted or food-borne diseases? We hope this book remains valuable to you for some time. Yes, the numbers of victims for various risks may change from year to year, and we will certainly learn more about some risks than we know now. But the nature of the consequences of alcohol consumption or radon exposure will stay the same. Years from now the use of caffeine, the prevalence of heart disease, the mechanics of the way radiation or lead or pesticides affects us, will all be pretty much the same. We also hope you find this book useful no matter where you live. While the numbers and exposure patterns we cite are focused on the United States, the details of most of the risks we explain are the same in Europe or Asia or South America. The effects of mercury, the science of genetic modification of food, the persistence of some chemicals in the environment, the way X rays work, are the same whether you live in Canada or France or Japan. We recognize that the relative scale of risks varies from place to place. The public health risk from cigarette smoke, for example, is higher in Europe, where more people smoke, than in the United States. Firearms risks are higher for U.S. residents than citizens of any other country. At the time of this writing, mad cow disease is a higher risk in some nations than others. So the data we use for exposure levels and numbers of victims, based on statistics for the United States, may well vary for citizens of different countries. But the general explanations of many of the risks we explore are applicable for anyone, anywhere.


Reviews:

WHAT IS RISK? Of all the wonders that I yet have heard, It seems to me most strange that men should fear; Seeing that death, a necessary end, Will come when it will come.
William Shakespeare, Julius Caesar An anonymous writer once observed, "To risk living is to risk dying." Risk is, indeed, inescapable. But just what is risk? How do you define it? To a stockbroker it means the prospect of losing, or making, money. Same thing for a person at the racetrack or at a blackjack table. For a skier or a bungee jumper or a skydiver, on the other hand, risk has more to do with physical than fiscal health. To the person taking a pill with known side effects, it"s about choice. To the person eating food with potentially harmful ingredients that aren"t listed on the label, it"s about no choice. At it"s simplest, risk is the idea that something might happen, usually something bad. But within that simple notion are some important components that you need to understand in order to have a better basis on which to make your personal risk judgments. You may be hoping that this book answers the common question we all have about most risks: "What are the chances that . . . ?" If you are like most people, you think that risk means probability, the likelihood that something will happen, as in "Your risk of dying from X is one in a million." But there is more to risk than just calculating the statistical chances of a certain outcome. There is also the issue of consequences, as in "The likelihood of a nuclear plant meltdown may be low, but it"s a risk because it"s disastrous if it does happen." A full definition of risk must take into account not just the probability of an outcome, but its severity. Generally, risk involves an outcome that is negative. You might say, "The odds of winning the lottery are . . ." but you wouldn"t say that winning the lottery is a risk. And the more severe the outcome, the higher we judge the risk to be. A complete definition of risk must also include the presence of a hazard, as in "That compound is a risk. It causes cancer in lab animals." If something to which we"re exposed isn"t hazardous, it isn"t a risk. We"re all exposed to a lot of cotton in the clothes we wear. So what. Which brings up the fourth major component of risk, exposure, as in "Flooding isn"t a risk. I live on a hilltop." If a substance is harmful to test subjects, but we"re never exposed to it, it doesn"t pose a risk. The risk of being eaten by a shark doesn"t exist in Kansas. A hazard can"t do you any harm if you are out of harm"s way. So a more complete way of thinking about risk might read: Risk is the probability that exposure to a hazard will lead to a negative consequence. It"s helpful to keep all these elements in mind when thinking about risk. Take out any one of those components, and the definition is incomplete. Each one involves characteristics that help you understand risks more completely and keep them in clearer perspective. As an illustration, let"s consider that dreaded common risk: ketchup. If we are exposed to ketchup, that exposure alone doesn"t make it a risk. As far as we know, ketchup isn"t a hazard, except for the chance of spilling some on your clothes. But let"s say somebody discovers that ketchup is hazardous. It still isn"t much of a risk if the consequence of exposure to this hazard is, say, an increased taste for pickles on your hamburger. The nature and severity of the consequence has a lot to do with judging whether a risk is big or small. But let"s say that you"re allergic to pickles, so anything that entices you to eat them could indeed be dangerous to your health. Ketchup still isn"t much of a risk if the probability of its leading to increased pickle consumption is one in a million. You may have exposure to a hazard, but the level of risk still depends on the likelihood, the chance, that a negative consequence might occur. In other words, we can make better judgments about how to think about risks if we keep in mind the ideas of hazard, exposure, consequence, and probability. These characteristics help to define and explain the risks in this book. Accordingly, most chapters are laid out as follows: The Hazard: Just what is the agent we"re talking about? (What is asbestos?) How does this hazard come to be in the world around us? (How does mercury get into our fish?) What is the biological or physical mechanism by which the hazard does its supposed harm? (How does radiation affect us?) The Range of Exposures: How are we exposed to this risk? Where? When? How do exposures vary over time, by location, or by population subgroup? The Range of Consequences: How much harm does the hazard do? In what ways? To how many people? To what kinds of people? Who is most at risk? Is the harm short-term or long-term, fatal or not? What is the probability of harm? How many people are injured or killed by the risk? Reducing Your Risk: In this section we offer some general suggestions about what you can do to minimize the risk we"re discussing. For More Information: Each chapter ends with a list of resources to provide you with more information. Perhaps the biggest risk we take as authors is offering our perspective and judgment of whether the risk is big or small, with visual guides at the beginning of each chapter. This estimate is our best effort to synthesize what we"ve learned on your behalf and to give you our opinion. You will find two "risk meters" in each chapter. One will offer our assessment of the general likelihood of exposure to hazardous levels, taking into account the factors of exposure and hazard from our definition. The other meter will indicate our assessment of the risk"s consequencesincluding severity and number of people affected. Here are a few examples of what you will see in each chapter. At the beginning of Chapter 1, "Accidents," the first risk meter will look like this: The upper bar indicates that the likelihood of exposure to accidents in a way that will probably cause harm is high. The lower bar indicates that the consequences of the risk of accidentsthe severity of the outcome and how many people suffer these consequencesare also high. But not quite as high as the first meter, because the majority of accidents are not fatal, so the severity of the consequences brings the rating down a bit. Here"s what the meter will look like for Chapter 35, "Radon." The likelihood of exposure to levels that will probably cause harm is, in general, pretty low. Lots of people are exposed, but the levels in most cases are fortunately not usually enough to cause harm. So there is exposure, but not to levels that present a hazard in a lot of cases. But since the consequences of radon exposure at levels high enough to do harm are potentially severe, and several thousand Americans a year suffer those consequences, the lower bar for radon takes account of both those factors. These meters require several cautions. First, they refer to the population as a whole. Your risk is almost certainly different from that of the general population because of your age, gender, genetics, income, education, location, and other factors that make you unique. These risk meters offer only a general reference to where we think the risk falls on the high-low scale. Second, these are estimates. They are not scientific. They are the result of our analysis of the information we"ve collected and are not statements of fact and truth. And since there is a lot of uncertainty about many of the risks in this book, the meters are ballpark estimates that offer only a general range of where we think the risk falls. That"s why we don"t give our ratings specific numbers between 0 and 10. (In Appendix 2 we discuss our thinking behind each of the ratings. The appendix does offer our ratings numerically, though some of them are given as a range rather than as one specific number.) Further, these risk gauges don"t take into account the benefits that come from the hazard being discussed. Air bags can be harmful, for example, but clearly they save many more lives than they take. Some people suffer serious side effects from vaccines, but vaccination"s benefits far outweigh the risks. We leave that risk-benefit accounting out of our judgment. Since our definition or risk presumes that most people think of risk as resulting in a negative consequence, that"s what we rate. Not every risk meter offers a ranking. Instead, a question mark acknowledges that some risks are too new or poorly studied to rate. As an example, for endocrine disrupters, the meter looks like this: In a few cases the risk meter will be blank. That"s because there is no specific hazard, so the first bar is irrelevant. A few of the risks we discuss, like cancer and heart disease, are really outcomes. That is, you"re not exposed to cancer or heart disease. You just end up with these illnesses as a result of other processes. But because they kill so many people, and are so often the result of exposure to many of the hazards we discuss, we think they deserve explanation in a book about risk. WHERE DO "THE FACTS" ABOUT RISK COME FROM?
"There is something fascinating about science. One gets such wholesale returns of conjecture out of such a trifling investment of fact."
Mark Twain As we try to judge what"s risky and what"s not, we look to science for answers. But even with all the facts that science can provide, much uncertainty remains, for a number of reasons. First, the sciences by which risk is investigatedtoxicology, epidemiology, and statistical analysisare inherently imprecise. Second, there are a lot of risk questions science simply hasn"t asked yet. New risks like using a cell phone while driving or eating genetically modified food haven"t been studied nearly enough for us to have all the answers. And third, even for risks that have been studied, the facts as we know them are constantly changing as scientific answers to one set of questions reveal more questions. A lot of information in this book comes from the findings of the three major risk sciences. It"s important for anyone trying to make informed judgments about risk to understand what these sciences can, and cannot, tell us.

Toxicology Most simply described, toxicology is the study of poisons. But because of that very definition, you can understand why toxicologists usually can"t test the agent they"re investigating on human subjects. So animals are used as surrogates. But toxicologists admit that they can"t say for sure what a compound will do in humans based on evidence of what it does in animals. As one toxicologist says, "With stuff that might kill people, animal testing, as imprecise as it is, is the best we can do. But despite what you might think of your boss or some people you don"t like, humans aren"t rats." Toxicologists don"t know which lab animal species serve as the best indicators of what would happen in people, nor do they know which species are better indicators for which kinds of hazards. So extrapolating from lab animals to humans is imprecise. As one example, cyclamate, an artificial sweetener, causes one type of liver tumor in only one species of rat, and then only in males, and doesn"t cause it in any other test animals. Yet test data from the experiments on those rats caused the food additive to be banned for human consumption. Another imprecision from toxicology arises because testing of lab animals often involves subjecting the animals to massive doses of an agent. In testing for carcinogenicity, animals routinely get doses, each day, far greater than you would be exposed to in your entire lifetime. Toxicologists call this dose the MTD, for "maximum tolerated dose." They use this technique when testing for cancer in order to maximize the chance that they"ll find any effect that might occur and that might not show up from a milder dose. Using these MTDs, toxicologists presume that if the substance they"re testing causes an effect at a high dose, it might cause the same effect at a lower dose. This approach seems like a rational way to deal with potentially dangerous chemicals and other agents; if high doses cause harm, assume that low doses might too. But sometimes the size of the dose is what"s really causing the harm. Think of aspirin, for example. One or two aspirin are fine. Too many will kill you. The standard toxicological approach of subjecting lab animals to high doses of a test compound can reveal subtle effects, but it can also produce misleading results. (Toxicological tests for noncancer health problems, like developmental hazard or cognitive impairment, don"t rely on the MTD approach. For these outcomes, scientists assume that higher levels cause worse effects and lower levels cause weaker effects, and below a certain level the hazard might not cause an effect at all. So they subject the test animals to varying doses to find the lowest one at which an effect occurs. Also, for these other health problems, scientists believe that once exposure to the agent stops the effect usually goes away. But for cancer, since just a one- time mutation of a single gene can create permanent changes to the DNA that causes the disease, toxicologists use the MTD method, a more conservative and precautionary approach.) A further imprecision arises in toxicology because in vivo tests in living lab animals, or in vitro tests of cells in a lab dish or beaker, isolate and test just one compound at a time. That"s a smart way to find out with precision whether that particular agent is hazardous. But in the real world we"re exposed to a stew of agents, and the mix can lead to different outcomes than exposure to any individual component. (Radon and smoking, for instance, apparently work synergistically and increase the risk of lung cancer more than the sum of one risk plus the other risk.) In addition, while the environment in the lab is stable and uniform, the real world is full of variables such as our environment, our health, our food, our emotional states, and our genetic makeup from one generation to the next and from one person to the next. These factors and many others affect how we react to a compound or circumstance. In short, while toxicology can tell us a lot about the biological hazard of a particular chemical or element or compound, it can"t tell us with absolute accuracy just what the substance being testedat high doses to another species in a controlled labwill do at lower doses to humans in the complicated real world. Epidemiology
When we can"t test a substance or hazard on people but we want to know whether it might be a threat to public health, we look around for circumstances in which people might already have been exposed. Studying what has happened, or is currently happening, to real populations in the real world, and trying to make sense of which hazards and exposures might be associated with which consequences, is the essence of epidemiology. Like toxicologists, epidemiologists readily acknowledge that their science is imprecise. Epidemiology can usually provide only associations, not absolute proof, that some particular exposure may be what"s causing some particular consequence. For example, in one kind of study epidemiologists investigate a specific small group of people who get sick. The book and movie A Civil Action, for instance, made famous the polluted drinking water in Woburn, Massachusetts.
A higher-than-expected number of cases of childhood leukemia showed up in just a few years in a small neighborhood. Epidemiologists investigated to find out what sources of exposure to potential hazards the neighbors shared. They discovered that one thing the neighbors had in common was that those who drank from a certain water supply had a higher rate of illness. Therefore, something about the water was the likely cause of the leukemia. They tested the water for chemicals suspected to cause that illness and estimated how much of the water people drank, for how long, and how polluted it was when people drank it. In the end, a peer-reviewed epidemiological study showed an association between how much of the well water pregnant mothers drank and the frequency of childhood leukemia in their offspring. The more they drank, the more likely it was that their children developed leukemia. But that"s not proof. Perhaps a couple of the neighbors were exposed to something else the researchers didn"t ask about. Maybe the researchers never detected something else in the well water. These other factors are known as "confounders," hidden clues that can muddy the epidemiological waters and lead to an inaccurate assumption that A caused B. Hidden confounders can never be completely ruled out. Epidemiologists can also do a different type of study, not just looking back in time at a small local group of people over just a few years but tracking a much larger population forward over longer periods of time. The famous Framingham Heart Study is an example of this kind of epidemiology, following an entire community over decades. Again, the researchers examine these wider populations for patterns in illnesses and exposures that suggest an association between the two. For instance, many epidemiological studies show that there is a strong association between air pollution increasing one day and hospital admissions for respiratory problems going up over the following several days, which suggests that the pollution is probably causing the respiratory problems. But again, that apparent association is not the smoking gun of absolute proof. Only when many long-term studies of different large populations repeatedly show the same thing, as with tobacco smoking and lung cancer, can epidemiology confidently say A causes B. This isn"t to suggest that the findings of epidemiology are weak or of little use in judging risks. In good epidemiological studies, researchers give the research subjects in-depth questionnaires about their health, their lifestyle, their diet, their social and economic characteristics, even their residential history (where they have lived and when), trying to rule out all confounders. They compare a group of people suffering some kind of health problem, like those families in Woburn, with other "control" groups, populations of similar size and socioeconomic status somewhere else, who presumably were not exposed to the same things. For the bigger long-term population studies, epidemiologists carry out multiple research programs in different places at different times to see if their results agree. With such techniques, epidemiologists can rule out every other possibility they can think of. They can become more and more certain of the associations they find. But, like toxicologists, they can rarely be completely sure. Statistical Analysis
In addition to the findings of toxicology and epidemiology, risk analysts also look for their clues among large sets of statistics. Those data collections are compilations of real-world information, on either morbidity (nonfatal health problems) or mortality (deaths). These databases can offer rich details, like how many people were injured or killed in motor vehicle accidents, categorized by speed, vehicle size, whether the victim was male, female, old, young, wearing a seat belt or not, and so on. There are data sets on hundreds of risks that offer information on the age, gender, and race of the affected population and the circumstances that led to the death or illness, such as the number of food poisoning cases connected with restaurants, or the number of workers murdered on the job. Other data collections provide risk analysts with information about hazardous materials emissions, local water or air pollution levels, or the presence of harmful chemicals in our blood or the food we eat. These details all offer insights about the hazard, exposure, consequence, and probability of various risks. But the numbers in these data collections usually suffer from some imprecision. Not everybody who suffers food poisoning after dining at a restaurant, for example, actually goes to a doctor to report his illness. Not every police officer fills out every last detail on every accident report. Not every factory keeps accurate, or honest, records of its emissions. And not every government information collection system gathers the information and enters it into its database accurately. Numbers are also subject to interpretation. Here"s an example. According to national motor vehicle crash statistics, drivers 75 years old or over are involved in four times as many fatal crashes as the average of all other age groups. But does that mean that elderly drivers are killing other people, or just that because of frail health they"re more likely to die themselves whenever they"re in a crash? You can"t tell by that statistic. The numbers don"t tell you everything you need to know. As Mark Twain said, "There are three kinds of lieslies, damned lies, and statistics." Finally, no matter how precise and narrow statistical categories are, they lump everybody in that category together. For example, federal motor vehicle crash statistics group data by age, gender, the day and time of crashes, and the kind of vehicle involved. So you can determine how many 15- to 24-year-old males were involved in crashes on Sundays at 5 p.m. in pickup trucks. As narrow as that seems, that"s still a large group of people and not everyone in it is the same. Individuals within that group have all sorts of differences in health, lifestyle, education, genetics, body size and shape, and on and on. Risk statistics are generalities, and by definition cannot specifically answer the question we all want answered: "What is the risk to me?" You will read a lot of numbers in this book. As we"ve stated, because you are unique none of those numbers will accurately and precisely answer your question. Risk numbers can be only a general guide. They give you a sense of which risks are bigger and which ones are smaller, and sometimes they can tell you which risks are higher or lower for the demographic groups to which you belong. But even risk numbers that define the categories as narrowly as possible still can"t calculate the risk for each unique individual. In sum, the sciences that supply the facts about risk, while growing more and more powerful, are still imprecise. They can provide us with valuable insights. But their results are uncertain and open to interpretation. There are very few unequivocal answers when it comes to defining and quantifying the risks we face. That"s why in this guide our approach is to offer information in ranges: the range of exposures, the range of consequences, and so forth. In addition to this scientific imprecision, sometimes we can"t tell whether a risk is big or small, or real at all, simply because it"s too new and hasn"t been studied enough. Our modern world presents us with many new technologies (cell phones) or processes genetic modification of food) or compounds (statin drugs to reduce cholesterol) that have profound benefits, but which also come with risks. Sometimes we are exposed to these technologies or processes or compounds before the risks have been adequately studied. In the professional and policymaking world of people who deal with risk, how we should handle this uncertainty is a hotly debated issue. Some people argue that we should thoroughly study anything that might pose a risk before we start to use it. The people on this side of the argument heed the advice of the eighteenth-century British politician Edmund Burke, who said, "Early and provident fear is the mother of safety." They suggest that we should adopt as a matter of law the "Precautionary Principle," the academic term for what most people think of as "Better Safe Than Sorry." These advocates argue that the best way to protect human and environmental health is to treat new compounds or technologies as guilty until proven innocent. They say that while we do this with some things, like new drugs, we don"t do it with others, like new industrial chemicals. Advocates of the Precautionary Principle say that we must apply this careful approach across the board. Others might subscribe to the advice of the American essayist Randolph Bourne, who wrote in his 1913 book Youth and Life, "We can easily become as much slaves to precaution as we can to fear. Although we can never rivet our fortune so tight as to make it impregnable, we may by our excessive prudence squeeze out of the life that we are guarding so anxiously all the adventurous quality that makes it worth living." These opponents of a sweeping Precautionary Principle argue that it would deny society many of the benefits of new technologies for years, even decades, until thorough scientific study can be completed. Those that argue against the Precautionary Principle also point out that almost anything carries some risk. Under the most rigorous application of the Precautionary Principle, these people claim, it would be hard to approve such things as motor vehicles or prescription drugs or vaccines. They argue that while it makes common sense to err on the side of caution, we should assess risks on a case-by- case basis, rationally weighing them against benefits. They say a blanket Precautionary Principle might deny society a public health advance that could save lives before all the scientific answers are in. There are also times when we think science has come up with "the" answer, and we"re reasonably certain about just how precautionary to be. And then things change. Even for risks that have been well studied, the facts are always evolving. We learn more and more every day. Between the time this book goes to press and the time you read it, our knowledge of the effects of hormone-disrupting chemicals will change. We"ll certainly know more about human genetics and the risk of some diseases. We"ll probably know more about the actual levels of particle pollution in the air we breathe. The statistical trends on established risks will have almost certainly shifted. Further, our world of rapid technological development means that new risks are being created, and new solutions are being found, at an accelerating rate. Many of the risks in this book were largely unknown just a few years ago. Researchers are constantly developing new technologies or drugs that reduce some risks while potentially creating others. And science itself changes and grows more powerful. We can detect chemicals in our blood or in the air at levels much lower than we could just a decade ago. Risks that were always out there are just now being revealed. And all of these discoveries and new risks and new solutions interact in highly unpredictable ways. Methyl tertiary-butyl ether, or MTBE, was added to gasoline to improve air quality. But that policy led to the pollution of drinking water. The certainty we want in order to know how to judge risks is a tough ideal to achieve in our modern, dynamic world. In short, our effort in this book to give you an accurate, reliable explanation of many of the risks you face is tantamount to shooting at a moving target. Which makes trying to score a bull"s-eye with everything we present here a very risky proposition. In an additional effort to ensure that what we present is accurate, thorough, and balanced, we asked experts to review each chapter. These reviewers were academics, scientists, doctors, government officials, engineers, risk assessors, and members of advocacy groups like the American Cancer Society and the Union of Concerned Scientists. We asked them to check our facts, to correct mistakes, to point out omissions, to clarifyand for feedback on whether we had provided a thorough and balanced overview of the issue. Their input was immensely valuable. But we take full responsibility for the final product. And the reviewers had nothing at all to do with the risk ratings in each chapter. We offer the names and backgrounds of the reviewers at the end of each chapter.

WHERE DO OUR FEARS ABOUT RISK COME FROM?
"People are disturbed, not by things, but by the view they take of them."
Epictetus As we wrote earlier, the facts about risk are only part of the matter. Ultimately we react to risk with more emotion than reason. We take the information about a risk, combine it with the general information we have about the world, and then filter those facts through the psychological prism of risk perception. What often results are judgments about risk far more informed by fear than by facts.
The terrorist attacks on the World Trade Center in New York and on the Pentagon and the subsequent anthrax attacks in the fall of 2001 are an example. Many of us were afraid, and rightly so. But some people responded by driving to a distant destination rather than flying, even though the facts clearly showed that flying remained the far safer mode of transportation, even after September 11. Some people bought guns, raising their risks from firearms accidents far more than reducing their risk of being attacked by a terrorist. Many people took broad-spectrum antibiotics even though they had no evidence that they had been exposed to anthraxbut they didn"t get an annual flu shot. Do these judgments make sense? Are they rational? Not based simply on the facts. But this is how humans respond to risk . . . with our hearts as well as our heads.
The psychological study of this phenomenon, known as "risk perception," explains why our fears often don"t match the facts. It is perhaps the biggest reason why writing this book is a risky affair. We"re confident that as you read this guide, your interpretation of what we say about various risks will differ from ours, and from some of your friends or family or neighbors. Same facts. Different interpretations. Despite our efforts to be neutral, many of the issues we write about are highly emotional and trigger powerful risk perception responses that all but guarantee that you might not like, or agree with, all of what you"re about to read. We think it"s valuable to understand what researchers have learned about risk perception because it might help you understand your own reactions to risk a little better. Risk Perception
Humans tend to fear similar things, for similar reasons. Scientists studying human behavior have discovered psychological patterns in the subconscious ways we "decide" what to be afraid of and how afraid we should be. Essentially, any given risk has a set of identifiable characteristics that help predict what emotional responses that risk will trigger. Here are a few examples of what are sometimes called "risk perception factors." Most people are more afraid of risks that are new than those they"ve lived with for a while. In the summer of 1999, New Yorkers were extremely afraid of West Nile virus, a mosquito-borne infection that killed several people and that had never been seen in the United States. By the summer of 2001, though the virus continued to show up and make a few people sick, the fear had abated. The risk was still there, but New Yorkers had lived with it for a while. Their familiarity with it helped them see it differently. Most people are less afraid of risks that are natural than those that are human-made. Many people are more afraid of radiation from nuclear waste, or cell phones, than they are of radiation from the sun, a far greater risk. Most people are less afraid of a risk they choose to take than of a risk imposed on them. Smokers are less afraid of smoking than they are of asbestos and other indoor air pollution in their workplace, which is something over which they have little choice. Most people are less afraid of risks if the risk also confers some benefits they want. People risk injury or death in an earthquake by living in San Francisco or Los Angeles because they like those areas, or they can find work there. Most people are more afraid of risks that can kill them in particularly awful ways, like being eaten by a shark, than they are of the risk of dying in less awful ways, like heart diseasethe leading killer in America. Most people are less afraid of a risk they feel they have some control over, like driving, and more afraid of a risk they don"t control, like flying, or sitting in the passenger seat while somebody else drives. Most people are less afraid of risks that come from places, people, corporations, or governments they trust, and more afraid if the risk comes from a source they don"t trust. Imagine being offered two glasses of clear liquid. You have to drink one. One comes from Oprah Winfrey. The other comes from a chemical company. Most people would choose Oprah"s, even though they have no facts at all about what"s in either glass. We are more afraid of risks that we are more aware of and less afraid of risks that we are less aware of. In the fall of 2001, awareness of terrorism was so high that fear was rampant, while fear of street crime and global climate change and other risks was low, not because those risks were gone, but because awareness was down. We are much more afraid of risks when uncertainty is high, and less afraid when we know more, which explains why we meet many new technologies with high initial concern. Adults are much more afraid of risks to their children than risks to themselves. Most people are more afraid of asbestos in their kids" school than asbestos in their own workplace. You will generally be more afraid of a risk that could directly affect you than a risk that threatens others. U.S. citizens were less afraid of terrorism before September 11, 2001, because up till then the Americans who had been the targets of terrorist attacks were almost always overseas. But suddenly on September 11, the risk became personal. When that happens, fear goes up, even though the statistical reality of the risk may still be very low. People who first learn about these risk perception patterns often remark on how much sense they seem to make. It"s little wonder. These are deeply ingrained patterns, probably ancient behaviors imprinted in us over millions of years of evolution. Long before we had our modern thinking brain, long before humans or primates even developed, only organisms that could recognize and successfully respond to danger survived and evolved. In Darwinian terms, these affective, "irrational" ways of protecting ourselves are adaptive. They help us preserve the species. Evolution selects for this type of behavior. That belief is supported by the fact that these patterns of risk perception cross cultures, age groups, genders, and other demographic groupings. There are some variations among individuals. Those variations make sense too because different people have different lives, different jobs, different family circumstances, different sets of experiences, different sets of values, and so on. Fearing a risk more if it involves children, for example, means parents will react differently from, say, teenagers. What is frightening to you might not be to your friend. Neither of you is right or wrong. You just each have a unique perspective on the same statistics and facts. But risk perception research shows that underneath our individual differences, we share certain patterns of risk response. As we"ve written, this way of protecting yourself can be dangerous. What feels safe might actually be dangerous: driving instead of flying, antibiotics against anthrax instead of flu shots, arming yourself against a phantom risk. We explain risk perception, therefore, to help you understand the psychological roots of how we all respond to risk. That might help you understand your own concerns and put the risk issues in your life into clearer perspective. THE RISKS WE INCLUDE, AND THOSE WE DON"T
This book does not include or omit risks based on whether they are "real." As we have mentioned, whether a risk is real is ultimately something you will decide for yourself. Rather, what follows in this book are many of the major risks you might want to learn about. In selecting what to include, we made no distinction among those that have been largely debunked (radiation from microwave ovens), those that are new (cancer from cell phones), those that remain poorly understood (hormone-disrupting chemicals), or those that have been well studied (electrical and magnetic fields from electricity lines and appliances).We include risks with high likelihoods, such as food poisoning, and low likelihoods, including cancer from pesticides on food. That said, we do not include other risks. For example, we do not explore risks to the environment that don"t have direct implications for human health. While risks like climate change, acid rain, and destruction of wetlands or forests all impact the biosphere on which we depend, they do not have a direct and immediate connection to human health. We also do not include many of the risks that arise in detailed medical care. We do discuss medical errors in general as a category of risk, and we also explore some broad medical issues, such as vaccines or antibiotic resistance. We write about some health risks, like heart disease, cancer, and obesity. But we do not explain risks from drug reactions, the relative risk of one form of medical treatment over another, or many other specific medical risks. These risks are so unique to each individual that to discuss them in a book about risk in general might in fact be dangerous to the reader. Nor do we discuss the risks of developing health problems because of genetic predisposition, which has more to do with susceptibility than cause. We don"t include many things that you might think of as risks, but that are actually outcomes. Stroke and diabetes, for instance, are end results, the outcomes of natural biological processes or, in some cases, of exposure to a hazard. This book deals with the hazards to which we are exposed, because our actions have bearing on these parts of the risk equation. By the time we"re faced with an outcome, it"s too late. Still, we do include a few outcomesheart disease, cancer, obesitybecause they are such major killers and such common outcomes to many of the hazards we write about that we felt a general explanation of these issues would help. We also don"t go into detail about the risks of crime, a complex and unique set of issues. Some basic crime statistics are included in Appendix 1 in the back of the book. We leave for this appendix those risks that, while of interest, really don"t need much of an explanation, like the risk of being hit by lightning, being killed in a plane crash, of snakebite or bee sting or shark attack. We also list in that appendix the statistics for some common causes of death and injury, such as stroke or homicide, diabetes or drowning, asthma or Alzheimer"s disease. This appendix is simply a numerical listing of how many Americans suffer these outcomes each year. It is not a chart of your individual risk. As the historian Edward Gibbon wrote, "The laws of probability. So true in general. So fallacious in particular." Your individual risk depends on dozens of factors unique to your lifestyle, genetics, socioeconomic characteristics, and so on. In the end, we hope we came up with a list, and an approach, that offers a review of what is known, and what is not known, about most of the risks that most people care about. We have tried to offer information in a neutral way, keeping matters simple by culling the essentials from the mountains of information on each risk. At the same time, we have rigorously pursued accuracy, and we"ve tried to offer some context and richness of detail. We hope this guide provides useful information that will help put the risks in your life in perspective. We hope it helps you lead a healthier, safer, less worried life. Copyright 2002 by David Ropeik and George Gray. Reprinted by permission of Houghton Mifflin Company.

This is the scientific facts without the hype
This is an excellent book on the subject of risk analysis focused on 48 specific risks we encounter in everyday life. The book is divided in three parts. Part I describes mainly discretionary or behavioral risks. These consist mainly of risks we choose to incur such as smoking, drinking alcohol, and drinking coffee. Part II focuses on environmental risks. These are risks that we bear, and for the most part can't avoid such as water and air pollution. Part III describe Medical related risks. As the authors specify these are often more outcomes than risks. For instance, cancer and heart disease are not direct risks, they are outcome of a combination of deficient nutrition, bad lifestyle habits (lack of exercise), and inherited genes. The authors make an excellent effort to come up with the most current and objective scientific knowledge. They avoid all the hype in the media that may exaggerate or understate various risks. After reading this excellent book, it is interesting to notice that by far the biggest risks to our health and survival are the behavioral risks or the risks we choose to undertake. These include smoking, drinking, obesity, and also sun tanning. These risks are far greater than pesticides, water pollution, air pollution, electro magnetic fields, and radiation from cellular phones. Thus, the authors do a good job to strengthen our common sense based on scientific evidence instead of going crazy due to misinformation by the media. The book is excellent for several reasons. First, the authors have a solid scientific background themselves. Second, they fully recognized that no matter how smart you are, you just can't be the number one expert in everything. Thus, each of the chapters (dedicated to any one of the specific 48 risks) has been fully reviewed by one or more of the top authorities in the relevant field covered. Therefore, the book does not reflect just their opinions. In essence, each of their risk analysis has been peer-reviewed by the top specialists. Third, they provide excellent reference at the end of each section to credible websites where you could further research specific issues if you cared too. Fourth, they came up with a self explanatory Risk Meter that is a visual representation of the specific exposure to a certain risk, and severity of consequence if you are exposed to this same risk. Thus, very quickly you can get a read on how serious a specific risk is right at the beginning of each chapters. Fifth, in the Appendix 2, the authors summarize their opinions on all 48 risks. So, if you just wanted to know the bottom line on a series of rather complex risks, you could quickly refer to this Appendix, and in seconds you can figure how material these risks are to yourself. This is definitely an excellent reference book. It is probably not the type of book you read in a sequential fashion cover to cover. Only the Ben Stiller character (a neurotic risk assessment specialist working for a life insurance company) in the comedy "Along Came Polly" would. I admit, I am like this character, and I managed to read about 29 of the 48 risks straight through. But, that is just me. I am a bit nuts about that stuff. You'll probably get a lot more by referring to the book whenever the media, or the experience of a friend or relative triggers within you a health-risk issue you want to know more about.


<< Book  Book >>

More books in the category:
Risks, Statistics and Correlations